Australian Adoptee's Site

ATTACHMENT, FAMILY SEPARATION AND MENTAL HEALTH: MAKING CONNECTIONS

Home
Breaking News
Etiology of adopted children
Complications In Therapy
Adoption and Eating Disorders
Patterns of Psychiatric Disorder in Adopted Girls
Overrepresentation of Adopted Children
Adoptive Rage
An Individual at Risk?
Psychodynamics
Psychological Development
Attachment & Separation
ATTACHMENT AND IDENTITY
Veto Victims
Adoption and Adult Attachment Security
Reunification
A Personal Response
Mental Health of Person Affected by Separation
Restorying Disrupted Identities
The Separated Child
Senate Inquiry into Forced Adoption
Youth Suicides
About Us
The Color of Difference
The Damaged Adopted Child
Adoptee Trauma
Favorite Links
Contact Us

aussieflag.jpg

ATTACHMENT, FAMILY SEPARATION AND MENTAL HEALTH: MAKING CONNECTIONS by Timothy Keogh

 

Department of Medicine, University of Sydney Australia

Abstract

The aim of this paper is to discuss the relevance of attachment theory to the needs of people who are in care or who have experienced care as a result of family separation or loss. Whilst the significant emotional and psychological consequences of family separation for relinquishing mothers and families are acknowledged, the focus of the paper concerns the mental health aspects of those placed in various forms of care following separation or loss. The paper will review some of the links between family separation, which results in adoption, foster care, and institutionalized care and their mental health outcomes and attempt to demonstrate how attachment theory can assist in understanding these outcomes.  The paper will discuss some preliminary data concerning associations between disruptive experiences in the backgrounds of adult offenders in the New South Wales prison system and their mental health and recidivism.

Respect and consideration of those who have spent time in care and those who have relinquished children is implicit in the material to be presented along with an awareness that some of the material may be painful for those who have had such experiences.  

 

Introduction

There have been a variety of studies, which have demonstrated how disruptions to attachment and bonding can adversely impact on emotional and psychological development (Fonagy, 2001; Morton & Frith, 1995; Sperling & Bermann, 1994).  Family separation and loss experiences have been clearly identified as a risk factor for the development of mental health problems in childhood and adulthood (Fraser, 1997).  Although biology, temperament and experiences all play important roles, children who have had interrupted relationships with primary caregivers are more likely to have compromised mental health. (Folman, 1996).

 

Separation and loss can be traumatic and its impact depends on the circumstances of the separation or loss (Keogh, 1998).  The work of Van der Kolk  (1996) and others (Glaser, 1998) has shown how the timing and impact of trauma can lead to additional and sometimes devastating psychological and biological consequences.  Examples of traumatic family separation are revealed in the disturbing accounts of survivors of the Stolen Generation.

 

In the case of family separation or permanent loss resulting in care placement, the child’s age, the number of placements, the chronicity of the separation and the trauma associated with it have all been shown to significantly determine the mental health impact of the original separation or loss experience.

 

A WHO expert committee (1997) set up to review issues concerning child mental health and psychological development noted that the continuity of relationship to parent figures is especially important in the first few years of life. They also indicated that children are most at risk when they experience multiple changes of parent figures or when they are reared in institutions with many attendants who have no special responsibility for individual children.  They concluded that there are significant dangers to the emotional and psychological well-being of children who are delayed in their adoption process, to children who are taken in and out of institutions and foster care, or to those who are left to experience an impersonal institutional upbringing.

 

Further, there is now a large literature, which has documented the particular vulnerability of infants to the stress associated with separation, loss or insufficient care in their first years of life.  Glaser (1996) has summarized a considerable amount of this research, which demonstrates the potentially traumatic effect of such experiences on the growth of the brain.  Damage to the brain from such experiences not only compromises brain development itself, but such compromised brain functioning can also interact with later adverse developmental experiences adding to their negative impact and predict a greater likelihood of poor mental health adjustment in a later life. 

 

Society is still coming to grips with the reality that separation and loss experiences, which when compounded by poor subsequent attachment opportunities, can severely compromise a child’s future.

 

Institutionalisation

The adverse effects of institutionalisation on mental health have been identified for some time (Spitz & Wolf, 1945) along with evidence, that there are differential mental health effects associated with institutional care, foster care and adoption (REF**).

 

There has documented a litany of the adverse effects of institutionalisation which have included increased susceptibility to infectious morbidity, poor nutrition and growth and retarded cognitive development.  The risk of physical and sexual abuse is such settings has also become painfully clear (REF**).

 

There are also numerous accounts of the sexual and other abuses that institutionalized children have experienced which would have compounded their already adverse developmental milieu (REFS**).  Sequelae of sexual abuse include specific mental health difficulties which are related to the extent and nature of the abuse can encompass severe psychopathology including dissociative states and borderline personality disorder (REFS**).  Frank et al  (1996) recently noted that it significantly increases the likelihood of that children will grow up psychiatrically and emotionally impaired.

 

Goldfarb (1946) following on from the studies of Spitz and Woolfe (1945) demonstrated that there were five symptom clusters of psychological problems that persisted in institutionalized children into adolescence.  These were: hyperactivity and disorganization, indiscriminate demands for affection and attention, superficiality of relationships, the absence of normal anxiety in reaction to failure or rebuke and social regression.

 

Subsequently, Tizard et al (1975) followed the emotional and behavioural development of 40 children who were place in care.  They noted a number of features of such children, which indicated impaired capacity for attachment manifested in their inability to turn to peers for emotional support, and to achieve friendship.  They concluded that compared to children who were adopted or returned to impoverished families that the institutionalized children were ten times more likely to develop such problems.  Rutter and  Quinn(***) also replicated such findings with a cohort of young women and showed that although later positive experiences might moderate some of the earlier adverse ones, compared to a control group institutionalized girls as women were much more likely to have serious emotional adjustment problems which interfered with their capacity for emotional well-being. 

 

 

 

Foster care and adoption

Mental health sequale have also been associated with foster care or adoption (Ref**).  Adoption per se cannot be said to contribute to poor metal health outcome because it may also represent a protective factor by which children can build up resilience to mental health problems.  This is an important caveat in relation to foster care and adoption and their impact on mental health.  This relates to research which has shown that resilience to mental health problems can build up if a child has available to them a constant and consistent alternative attachment which such experiences often provide.

 

Indeed, Valiant (REF**) argued that the best single predictor for a long term positive mental health outcome in such children is the chance to form on good relationship with someone, not necessarily a parent or a relative. 

 

Positive re-attachment opportunities are therefore pivotal in increasing the potential for better mental health outcomes.  Also the child’s age, delays in fostering, the number of foster placements and matching of the child’s development needs with a suitable foster parent have proven to be especially predictive of better mental health outcome (REFS**). 

 

In the U.S. however, children are currently entering foster care at increasingly earlier ages, when as previously noted, they may be at most risk to adverse impact on their brain development which combined with  bonding and attachment disruptions can have profound implications for their future mental health (REFS***).

 

The American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (REF**) in a report on foster care has noted that fostered children commonly blame themselves and experience guilt about removal from their parents. They also note that such children often want to return the parents even if they were abused by them, and feel unwanted if they have to wait for a long time for a foster parent.  Such children also have mixed emotions about attaching to foster parents and have feelings of insecurity and uncertainties about their future.   Many of these experiences become the themes in their adult psycic life.  Such children as adults often experience feelings of aloneness and unworthiness that are deeply felt and can interfere with taking advantage of everyday opportunities for happiness that many other people take for granted.   As a group they are at risk for later depression, substance abuse, and criminality and can have relationship difficulties associated with the various forms of insecure attachment (Miller et al 2000)

 

Being aware of the difference of a child’s sense of time and taking into account the age and developmental needs of a child or infant are crucial determinants in facilitating their adjustments to the stresses associated with being fostered (REF**).

 

As a group adopted children appear to have more mental health problems than non-adopted children.  There a variety of factors, which impact on this outcome including the experiences prior to adoption, delays in the adoption process, and the quality of the attachments, made to adoptive parent(s).  Notwithstanding this Miller et al (1996) were able to replicate a study conducted by Warren (1994) which demonstrated that adopted adolescents were more likely to receive mental health services that non adopted adolescents. These findings corroborate the findings of a meta analyses of data concerning a variety of psychological and behavioral outcomes by Wierzbicki (1993) who showed adopted children showed consistent negative differences on all measures compared to non adopted children.  Slapp, Huang and Goodman (2001) have also demonstrated that adoption is a risk factor for attempted suicide during adolescence.  They found that compared to non-adopted children they were more likely to have depressive symptoms, have a poor self-image, be involved in more risk taking behaviors and have less of a sense of connectedness in their significant relationships.

 

Distilling many of the above-mentioned findings concerning the mental health sequalae of family separation it becomes clear that disruptions to attachment are of key to the resultant mental health outcomes.  An understanding of attachment research and theory therefore appears helpful in understanding the processeswhich appear to underpin these outcomes. 

 

Attachment theory

The fact that relationship or attachment with caregivers centrally involved in a child’s upbringing mediate development is a concept central to early intervention strategies and to the risk and resilience literature (Fraser 1997). This literature has identified categories of factors, which can either influence, the development of maladaptive behaviours or afford some protection against them. 

Attachment Theory (Fonagy 2001) in particular posits that secure attachment with caregivers fundamentally mediates psychological adjustment.  Bowlby (1958) had suggested that security and relatedly attachment were prime motivators in humans and saw secure attachment as an important precursor to stable adult relations.

Bowlby (1958) can be credited for having been the first theorist and clinician to articulate the importance of attachment to the understanding of human behaviour.  He believed that babies and infants had an evolutionary related need to ensure their safety by establishing proximity to with significant adults in their environment (usually their parents).

Bowlby specifically believed that children make internalized representations of these of the basic style of the relationships with their parents and that these internal working models become a template, which guides their relationships with others. He suggested that these models are built up around a relatively small and simplistic set of abstractions from these experiences.  These interpersonal transactions consolidate into internal working models during the period 9 - 18 months of age. This is also that time during which ‘object permanence’ (i.e. a stable psychological representation of another person) crystallises in the infant (Piaget 1954).

The development of Bowlby’s thinking resulted from a series of studies undertaken at the Tavistock Clinic in London in which a large number of babies’ reactions were observed where their contact with their primary caregivers was subject to interference and separations. (Bowbly 1971).

Ainsworth and her colleagues (1987) subsequently developed a methodology to explore these findings.  She developed a technique that became known as the 'strange situation' experiment in which a child was brought into the strange situation of a social laboratory and exposed to a graded series of mild disturbances to their contact with caregivers and culminating the caregivers brief departure.

They studied a large number of mother infant dyads and developed a typology of children’s’  attachment styles. The typology described children as being either securely, avoidantly or ambivalently attached.  Subsequently a disorganized category of attachment style was added (Main and Hess 1990). 

In the majority of cases she observed that children would react in a way that suggested that they could be characterized as ‘secure’.  These children manifested distress when the parent left but was relieved when the parent returned.  They demonstrated more concentration when they are at play, and had greater social competence.  They also displayed more positive affect and had greater ego resilience.

In contrast the ‘avoidant’ attachment style was characterized by distress during the separation from caregivers and by a lack of acknowledgment or rejection during the reunion phase.  The ‘ambivalent’ style was characterized by high level of distress during the separation and by a mixed approach and rejection during the reunion.

With the ‘disorganised’ attachment style during reunion with their caregiver, these infants exhibited disorganized and contradictory behaviour.  This included approaching the caregiver with the head averted, falling to the floor on approach to the caregiver and a kind of frozen response whereby they seem stuck to the floor and unable to approach the caregiver.  This attachment style is often more directly the consequence of abuse experiences of various kinds. The internal representations of these children are chaotically organized and often feature violent images (Main & Cassidy, 1988).  It is therefore not surprising that this category has been shown to predict the development of aggressive behavior.

The common underpinning issue in all these insecure attachment styles is that the basic anxiety is related to a feared loss of the (object) parent.  Such experience according to Bowlby (1971) was primarily mediated by the caregiver’s capacity for empathy or empathic attunement as Stern (1985) has referred it to.

 Crittenden (1996) has developed further sub-categories to this basic typology using the dimensions of cognition and affect.  She saw that the attachments in a child’s life is the 'scaffolding’ which allows the mind to interact with reality.

Zeanah, Bakshi, Boris and Lieberman (2000), and Zeanah Boris and Lieberman (2000) have also extended categories of attachment in infants and articulated secure base distortions that include categories of disordered attachment with: 1. Self-Endangerment, 2. Inhibition and 3. hypervigilance. 

Also pertinent is a schema of adult attachment styles developed by Bartholomew (1990), which built on the work of Hazan and Shaver (1987) who had proposed three categories of adult attachment roughly paralleling Ainsworth’s (1987) childhood categories. They proposed  ‘secure’, ‘ambivalent’ and  ‘avoidant’ categories of attachment in adults.

Bartholomew’s schema proposes four categories of attachment in adults as can be seen in diagram II

 

 

 

 

 

A Four Group Model of  Adult Attachment (Bartholomew 1990)

 

 

                                Model of Self   (Dependence)

 

Model of Other (avoidance)

 

                   Positive (Low)                                 Negative (High)

                                                    

 

 

 

 

Positive (Low)

 

 

 

 

                    Secure

 

 

 

 

                  Preoccupied

 

 

 

 

Negative (High)

 

 

 

 

                Dismissing

 

 

 

 

                      Fearful

Bartholomew  described four broad types of adult attachment as follows:

1.      Firstly, a secure autonomous attachment style, which is associated with an expectation of relationship permanence and a positive view about oneself and others. Such individuals do not generally worry about being alone or have worries about being accepted by others

2.      Secondly, a dismissing attachment style in which the individual is comfortable without close emotional relationships but where it is important for the individual to that they are not dependent on others. It is other s who are viewed negatively rather than the self.

3.      Thirdly, a preoccupied style which is characterised by an anxious and ambivalent approach to others.  Individuals with this attachment style experience others as reluctant to get close and worry about their self worth.

4.  Finally, a fearful style which is charcterised by mistrust of others and a worry about getting hurt if others are allowed to get close.

West and Keller (1994) have noted that the defensive attachment patterns results in defensive styles that represent unresolved mourning of the longed for but never experienced a tender care giving relationship with usually the parent.  They claim that such unresolved feelings always result in the renunciation of authentic relatedness with others in favour of a detachment from their attachment emotions, which cause them to adopt one of a number of defensive styles. 

In the last ten to fifteen years there has also been significant progress in linking attachment to the development of psychopathology (del Carmen and Huffman 1996; Boris, Wheeler, Heller and Zeanah 2000). Substantial progress has also been made linking insecure attachment and depression (e.g. Cummings and Cicchetti 1990; Lyons-Ruth 1996).

Gender differences have also been demonstrated with males manifesting risks of criminality violence substance abuse and occupational instability whilst women with the a sequalae of insecure attachment are more likely to feature depression, suicidal behavior, multiple somatic complaints, substance abuse and sometimes become involved in prostitution REF**).  Not surprisingly the care of children of insecurely attached parents can be compromised. 

Fonagy (1997) has argued that disruption to attachment leads to a failure to mentalize about experience and that this lack of a self reflectively ability results in a need to deal with psychological distress physically.  This is similar to Freud’s (1918) original idea of motor discharge in the absence of mature defences.  From this perspective aggression can be seen as a defence to safeguard the self from fantasies and thoughts which the self is not able to protect itself through mental manipulation (Fonagy & Target 1995).

Along with findings of the temporal stability of insecure attachment and its relationship to mental health these findings shed light on the ‘why’ of family separation and loss.  That is, what it is about family separation and loss, that links it to adverse mental health outcome

The development of conduct problems and criminality and its links to attachment is currently a burgeoning area of research.  Kosky (1992) for example has showed juvenile offenders to have high rates of psychological morbidity and mental health.  Adult offenders also as have high rates of mental health problems (REFS**).  These offenders often have high rates of care experiences in there backgrounds suggesting that such experiences link to their mental health outcomes which encompass deficits which directly link to their propensity to offend (Keogh, 2000).

For example Rosentein and Horowitz (1996) have shown that adolescents with a ‘dismissive’ attachment style rely on an attachment strategy that minimizes distressing thoughts and affects associated with rejection by the attachment figure.  They specifically found that adolescents with dismissing attachment patterns were more likely to have self-reported anti-social personality traits.  Thus insecure attachment has its link to criminality through mental health and psychological problems and related behaviors.

Fonagy (1998) has marshaled a significant body of evidence, which has demonstrated the likely causative link between disrupted attachment and the development of criminology. There is abundant evidence that criminality is associated with insecure attachment and more serious crime with non-attachment (Fonagy, Target, Steele & Steele 1997)

Violence and crime were for Bowlby (1946) disorders of attachment. In these disorders the criminal acts against others are permitted through a lack of concern, which is a result of the inhibition of bonding.

 

Preliminary data from an Australian inmate population

The following are some preliminary findings from a collaborative research project that I am involved in with the Corrections Health Service (Dr Tony Butler and Mr Simon Quilty), and  Dr. Joanna Penglase, Ms. Katherine McFaralne and Mr. John.   This research arose from an Inmate Health Survey conducted by the Corrections Health service in NSW led by Dr. Tony Butler.  The current research focuses on data sets that capture experiences that were disruptive to attachment in the backgrounds of adult offenders who were incarcerated at the time of the survey.

557 of total 995 inmates who had completed the inmate health survey were selected for study.  This smaller group were those who had also completed the CIDI (***), a research instrument which measures mental health indices.

The composition of this group sampled is detailed in Table 1 and shows the large proportion of indigenous Australians in the sample is an indication of their over-representation the incarcerated population.

Table 2 shows the overall rate of any mental disordered a category of ICD 10 which is an international classificatory system for mental disorders.  The cohort examined also had an overall 12% of attempted self-harm. (Table 3)

Data concerning the care background of these inmates (see Table 4) showed that 20 % of the total group had been placed in care before the age of 16 years with rate for women even higher (29%).   Table 5 shows the main type of care the group experienced and has you can see 15 % had experienced institutional care.

Table 6 revels the percentage breakdown of the number of times this group had been in care and it can be seen that a significant number have experienced multiple acre placements.

In the light of the previously mentioned research the age at which the group was first placed in care is also significant and Table 7 shows a large number had been placed in care before the age of 3 years.

The total time spent in care (Table 8) also revealed that a significant percentage of the group had spent more than five years in care.  This correlates with the findings presented in table 9 which is the amount of time in total that was spent with parents care before the age of 16 years.

These data reveal a strong correlation between mental health problems and time spent in care.

The analysis of the data to date has shown (see Table 10) that all the listed mental health indicators are substantially higher for those inmates who had had the most disruptions to their attachment and relatedly who had spent the most time in care.  Those who had the most disruptions to their attachment were also more likely to be a recidivist (see Table 11).

Taken together these data corroborate the body of attachment research data, which show a clear association between insecure attachment and mental health and insecure attachment and criminality

They also suggest a link between severely disrupted attachment occasioned by family separation and subsequently being placed in care, mental health problems and criminality where the mental health problems apparel to mediate the propensity to offend.

Conclusion

The research data and extant research literature presented clearly documented adverse mental health sequalae, associated with family separation, loss and subsequent care experiences with differential effects on mental health linked to institutional care, foster care, and adoption.  Notwithstanding this, the opportunity to form and maintain a primary attachment subsequent to disruption to attachment, has emerged as the factor most relevant to mental health outcome.

The data presented confirm that insecure attachment which ensues in various from family separation and loss can result in a wide range of mental health problems which have in common varying degrees of difficulty in forming close reciprocal relationships.  Criminality in it various forms appears to be one common consequence of severely disrupted attachment and has a range of mental health problems associated with it that seem to underpin the propensity to offend.

There is a strong case for the body of research and knowledge concerning attachment to inform our approach the strategies that are invoked when children are separated or loose their primary caregivers or families.  It also needs to inform policies, programs and services for those, including offenders who are struggling with consequences of their developmental history.

The value of attachment theory as a compassionate and philosophically sound means of making connections about the data we have concerning family separation and mental health is strongly supported by the data presented.

The next challenge may be to take this approach forward in the form of useful policies and programs for those in need in a manner that stresses our attachments to each other in this society. 

*Timothy Keogh is a Clinical and Forensic Psychologist and Psychotherapist in private practice and has a special clinical interest in attachment disorder.  He is a former Director of Inmate Services and Programs (Department of Corrective Services) Director Psychological and Specialist Programs (Department Juvenile Justice) and Director, Inner City Mental Health Service.  Tim is currently involved in a range of attachment research with the University of Sydney and Corrections Health Service.

 

adoptee3_logo_.jpg

Origins sites are copyrite
This site is auspiced by Origins SPSA Inc